- 一级建造师考试
- 二级建造师考试
- 三支一扶
- 安全评价师考试
- 保险经纪资格考试
- 报关员资格考试
- 博士入学考试
- 成人高考
- 成人英语三级考试
- 程序员考试
- 出版专业资格考试
- 大学英语三级
- 大学英语四六级考试
- 单证员考试
- 导游证考试
- 电气工程师
- 电子商务设计师考试
- 房地产经纪人考试
- 房地产评估师考试
- 高级会计师资格考试
- 高考
- 高中会考
- 给排水工程师
- 公共英语等级考试
- 公务员考试
- 国际货运代理
- 国际内审师
- 国家司法考试
- 化工师
- 环境影响评价师
- 会计人员继续教育
- 会计职称考试
- 基金从业资格
- 计算机等级考试
- 计算机软件水平考试
- 监理工程师考试
- 教师招聘
- 教师资格
- 结构工程师考试
- 经济师考试
- 考研
- 空姐招聘
- 遴选
- 美术高考
- 普通话考试
- 期货从业资格
- 求职招聘
- 人力资源管理师
- 软件设计师考试
- 商务英语考试(BEC)
- 社会工作者职业水平考试
- 审计师考试
- 事业单位招聘
- 事业单位招聘
- 数据库系统工程师
- 特许公认会计师(ACCA)
- 同等学力
- 统计师考试
- 托福考试(T0EFL)
- 外贸跟单员考试
- 网络工程师考试
- 网络管理员考试
- 网络规划设计师考试
- 系统分析师考试
- 消防工程师
- 小升初
- 校园招聘
- 信息系统管理工程师考试
- 选调生考试
- 雅思考试
- 岩土工程师考试
- 医生招聘
- 艺术高考(艺考)
- 银行从业人员资格
- 银行招聘
- 英语翻译资格考试
- 营销师考试
- 造假工程师考试
- 证券从业资格考试
- 中考
- 注册安全工程师考试
- 注册测绘师考试
- 注册城市规划师考试
- 注册环保工程师考试
- 注册会计师考试
- 注册计量师考试
- 注册建筑师考试
- 注册税务师考试
- 注册资产评估师
- 专升本考试
- 专业英语四级八级考试
- 自考
- 安全员
- 跟单员
- 考试一本通
- 其它资料
2015 年 6 月英语六级真题(第 3 套)
Part I
Writing
(30 minutes)
Directions: For this part, you are allowed 30 minutes to write an essay
commenting on the saying“If you cannot do great things, do
small things in a great way.’’You can cite examples to illustrate
your point of view. You should write at least l50 words but no more
than200words.
Part II
Listening Comprehension
(30 minutes)
说明:六级真题全国共考了两套听力。本套(即第三套)的听力内容与第二套的完全一样
只是选项的顺序不一样而已,故在本套中不再重复给出。
Part III
Reading Comprehension
(40 minutes)
Section A
Directions:In this section, there is a passage with ten blanks. You are
required to select one word for each blank from a list of choices
given in a word bank following the passage. Read the passage
through care fully before making your choices. Each choice in the
bank is identified by a letter. Please mark the corresponding letter
for each item on Answer Sheet2 with a single line through the
centre. You may not use any of the words in the bank more than
once.
Questions 36 to 45 are based on the following passage.
Innovation, the elixir (灵丹妙药) of progress, has always cost people their
jobs. In the Industrial Revolution hand weavers were 36 aside by the
mechanical loom. Over the past 30 years the digitalrevolution has 37 many of
the mid-skill jobs that supported 20th-century middle-class life. Typists,ticket
agents, bank tellers and many production-line jobs have been dispensed with,
just as the weavers were.
For those who believe that technological progress has made the world a
better place, such disruption is a natural part of rising 38. Although innovation
kills some jobs, it creates new and better ones, as a more 39 society becomes
richer and its wealthier inhabitants demand more goods and services. A
hundred years ago one in three American workers was 40 on a farm. Today
less than 2% of them produce far more food. The millions freed from the land
were not rendered 41, but found better-paid work as the economy grew more
sophisticated. Today the pool of secretaries has 42, but there are ever more
computer programmers and web designers.
Optimism remains the right starting-point, but for workers the dislocating
effects of technology may make themselves evident faster than its 43. Even if
new jobs and wonderful products emerge, in the short term income gaps will
widen, causing huge social dislocation and perhaps even changing politics.
Technology’s 44 will feel like a tornado (旋风), hitting the rich world first,
but 45sweeping through poorer countries too. No government is prepared for
it.
A. benefits
B. displaced
C. employed
D. eventually
E) impact
F) jobless
G) primarily
H) productive
I) prosperity
J) responsive
K) rhythm
L) sentiments
M) shrunk
N) swept
O) withdrawn
Section B
Directions:In this section, you are going to read a passage with ten
statements attached to it. Eachstatement contains information
given in one of the paragraphs. Identify the paragraph from which
the information is derived. You may choose a paragraph more than
once. Each paragraph is marked with a letter. Answer the questions
by marking the corresponding letter on Answer Sheet 2.
Why the Mona Lisa Stands Out
[A] Have you ever fallen for a novel and been amazed not to find it on lists of
great books? Or walked around a sculpture renowned as a classic, struggling
to see what the fuss is about? If so, you’ve probably pondered the question a
psychologist, James Cutting, asked himself: How does a work of art come to
be considered great?
[B] The intuitive answer is that some works of art are just great: of intrinsically
superior quality. The paintings that win prime spots in galleries, get taught in
classes and reproduced in books are the ones that have proved their artistic
value over time. If you can’t see they’re superior, that’s your problem.
It’s an intimidatingly neat explanation. But some social scientists have been
asking awkward questions of it, raising the possibility that artistic canons (名作
目录) are little more than fossilised historical accidents.
[C] Cutting, a professor at Cornell University, wondered if a psychological
mechanism known as the “mere-exposure effect” played a role in deciding
which paintings rise to the top of the cultural league. Cutting designed an
experiment to test his hunch (直觉). Over a lecture course he regularly showed
undergraduates works of impressionism for two seconds at a time. Some of
the paintings were canonical, included in art-history books. Others were lesser
known but of comparable quality. These were exposed four times as often.
Afterwards, the students preferred them to the canonical works, while a
control group of students liked the canonical ones best. Cutting’s students had
grown to like those paintings more simply because they had seen them more.
[D] Cutting believes his experiment offers a clue as to how canons are formed.
He reproduced works of impressionism today tend to have been bought by
five or six wealthy and influential collectors in the late 19th century. The
preferences of these men bestowed ( 给 予 ) prestige on certain works, which
made the works more likely to be hung in galleries and printed in collections.
The fame passed down the years, gaining momentum from mere exposure as
it did so. The more people were exposed to, the more they liked it, and the
more they liked it, the more it appeared in books, on posters and in big
exhibitions. Meanwhile, academics and critics created sophisticated
justifications for its preeminence (卓越). After all, it’s not just the masses who
tend to rate what they see more often more highly. As contemporary artists
like Warhol and Damien Hirst have grasped, critics’ praise is deeply entwined
( 交 织 ) with publicity. “Scholars”, Cutting argues, “are no different from the
public in the effects of mere exposure.”
[E] The process described by Cutting evokes a principle that the sociologist
Duncan Watts calls “cumulative advantage”: once a thing becomes popular, it
will tend to become more popular still. A few years ago,Watts, who is
employed by Microsoft to study the dynamics of social networks, had a similar
experience to Cutting’s in another Paris museum. After queuing to see the
“Mona Lisa” in its climate- controlled bulletproof box at the Louvre, he came
away puzzled: why was it considered so superior to the three other Leonardos
in the previous chamber, to which nobody seemed to be paying the slightest
attention?
[F] When Watts looked into the history of “the greatest painting of all time”,
he discovered that, for most of its life, the“Mona Lisa”remained in relative
obscurity. In the 1850s, Leonardo da Vinci was considered no match for giants
of Renaissance art like Titian and Raphael, whose works were worth almost
ten times as much as the “Mona Lisa”. It was only in the 20th century that
Leonardo’s portrait of his patron’s wife rocketed to the number-one spot. What
propelled it there wasn’t a scholarly re-evaluation, but a theft.
[G] In 1911 a maintenance worker at the Louvre walked out of the museum
with the “Mona Lisa” hidden under his smock (工作服). Parisians were shocked
at the theft of a painting to which, until then, they had paid little attention.
When the museum reopened, people queued to see the gap where the “Mona
Lisa” had once hung in a way they had never done for the painting itself. From
then on, the “Mona Lisa” came to represent Western culture itself.
[H] Although many have tried, it does seem improbable that the painting’s
unique status can be attributed entirely to the quality of its brushstrokes. It
has been said that the subject’s eyes follow the viewer around the room. But
as the painting’s biographer, Donald Sassoon, dryly notes, “In reality the
effect can be obtained from any portrait.” Duncan Watts proposes that the
“Mona Lisa” is merely an extreme example of a general rule. Paintings, poems
and pop songs are buoyed (使浮起) or sunk byrandom events or preferences
that turn into waves of influence, passing down the generations.
[I] “Saying that cultural objects have value,” Brian Eno once wrote, “is like
saying that telephones have conversations.” Nearly all the cultural objects we
consume arrive wrapped in inherited opinion; our preferences are always, to
some extent, someone else’s. Visitors to the “Mona Lisa” know they are about
to visit the greatest work of art ever and come away appropriately impressed
—or let down. An audience at a performance of “Hamlet” know it is regarded
as a work of genius, so that is what they mostly see. Watts even calls the
preeminence of Shakespeare a “historical accident”.
[J] Although the rigid high-low distinction fell apart in the 1960s, we still use
culture as a badge of identity. Today’s fashion for eclecticism ( 折 中 主 义 )—“I
love Bach, Abba and Jay Z”—is, Shamus Khan, a Columbia University
psychologist, argues, a new way for the middle class to distinguish
themselves from what they perceive to be the narrow tastes of those beneath
them in the social hierarchy.
[K] The intrinsic quality of a work of art is starting to seem like its least
important attribute. But perhaps it’s more significant than our social scientists
allow. First of all, a work needs a certain quality to be eligible to be swept to
the top of the pile. The “Mona Lisa” may not be a worthy world champion, but
it was in the Louvre in the first place, and not by accident. Secondly, some
stuff is simply better than other stuff. Read “Hamlet” after reading even the
greatest of Shakespeare’s contemporaries, and the difference may strike you
as unarguable.
[L] A study in the British Journal of Aesthetics suggests that the exposure
effect doesn’t work the same way on everything, and points to a different
conclusion about how canons are formed. The social scientists are right to say
that we should be a little sceptical of greatness, and that we should always
look in the next room. Great art and mediocrity (平庸) can get confused, even
by experts. But that’s why we need to see, and read, as much as we can. The
more we’re exposed to the good and the bad, the better we are at telling the
difference. The eclecticists have it.
46. According to Duncan Watts, the superiority of the “Mona Lisa” to
Leonardo’s other works resulted from the cumulative advantage.
47. Some social scientists have raised doubts about the intrinsic value of
certain works of art.
48. It is often random events or preferences that determine the fate of a piece
of art.
49. In his experiment, Cutting found that his subjects liked lesser known works
better than canonical worksbecause of more exposure.
50. The author thinks the greatness of an art work still lies in its intrinsic
value.
51. It is true of critics as well as ordinary people that the popularity of artistic
works is closely associated with publicity.
52. We need to expose ourselves to more art and literature in order to tell the
superior from the inferior.
53. A study of the history of the greatest paintings suggests even a great work
of art could experience years of neglect.
54. Culture is still used as a mark to distinguish one social class from another.
55. Opinions about and preferences for cultural objects are often inheritable.
Section C
Directions:There are 2 passages in this section. Each passage is followed by
some questions or unfinished statements. For each of them there
are four choices marked A), B), C), and D). You should decide on the
best choice and mark the corresponding letter on Answer Sheet 2
with a single line through the centre.
Passage One
Questions 56 to 60 are based on the following passage.
The report from the Bureau of Labor Statistics was just as gloomy as
anticipated. Unemployment in January jumped to a l6-year high of 7.6
percent, as 598,000 jobs were slashed from US payrolls in the worst singlemonth decline since December, 1974. With 1.8 million jobs lost in the last
three months, there is urgent desire to boost the economy as quickly as
possible. But Washington would do well to take a deep breath before reacting
to the grim numbers.
Collectively, we rely on the unemployment figures and other statistics to
frame our sense of reality. They are a vital part of an array of data that we use
to assess if we’re doing well or doing badly, and that in turn shapes
government policies and corporate budgets and personal spending decisions.
The problem is that the statistics aren’t an objective measure of reality; they
are simply a best approximation. Directionally, they capture the trends, but
the idea that we know precisely how many are unemployed is a myth. That
makes finding a solution all the more difficult.
First, there is the way the data is assembled. The official unemployment
rate is the product of a telephone survey of about 60,000 homes. There is
温馨提示:当前文档最多只能预览 4 页,此文档共8 页,请下载原文档以浏览全部内容。如果当前文档预览出现乱码或未能正常浏览,请先下载原文档进行浏览。
发表评论(共0条评论)
下载需知:
1 该文档不包含其他附件(如表格、图纸),本站只保证下载后内容跟在线阅读一样,不确保内容完整性,请务必认真阅读
2 除PDF格式下载后需转换成word才能编辑,其他下载后均可以随意编辑修改
3 有的标题标有”最新”、多篇,实质内容并不相符,下载内容以在线阅读为准,请认真阅读全文再下载
4 该文档为会员上传,版权归上传者负责解释,如若侵犯你的隐私或权利,请联系客服投诉
点击加载更多评论>>